A gooey slice of pizza. A pile of crispy French fries. Ice cream dripping down a cone on a hot summer day. When you look at any of these foods, a specialized part of your visual cortex lights up, according to a new study from MIT neuroscientists.
This newly discovered population of food-responsive neurons is located in the ventral visual stream, alongside populations that respond specifically to faces, bodies, places, and words. The unexpected finding may reflect the special significance of food in human culture, the researchers say.
"Food is central to human social interactions and cultural practices. It's not just sustenance," says Nancy Kanwisher, the Walter A. Rosenblith Professor of Cognitive Neuroscience and a member of MIT's McGovern Institute for Brain Research and Center for Brains, Minds, and Machines. "Food is core to so many elements of our cultural identity, religious practice, and social interactions, and many other things that humans do."
The findings, based on an analysis of a large public database of human brain responses to a set of 10,000 images, raise many additional questions about how and why this neural population develops. In future studies, the researchers hope to explore how people's responses to certain foods might differ depending on their likes and dislikes, or their familiarity with certain types of food.
MIT postdoc Meenakshi Khosla is the lead author of the paper, along with MIT research scientist N. Apurva Ratan Murty. The study appears in the journal Current Biology.
Visual categories
More than 20 years ago, while studying the ventral visual stream, the part of the brain that recognizes objects, Kanwisher discovered cortical regions that respond selectively to faces. Later, she and other scientists discovered other regions that respond selectively to places, bodies, or words. Most of those areas were discovered when researchers specifically set out to look for them. However, that hypothesis-driven approach can limit what you end up finding, Kanwisher says.
"There could be other things that we might not think to look for," she says. "And even when we find something, how do we know that that's actually part of the basic dominant structure of that pathway, and not something we found just because we were looking for it?"
To try to uncover the fundamental structure of the ventral visual stream, Kanwisher and Khosla decided to analyze a large, publicly available dataset of full-brain functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) responses from eight human subjects as they viewed thousands of images.
"We wanted to see when we apply a data-driven, hypothesis-free strategy, what kinds of selectivities pop up, and whether those are consistent with what had been discovered before. A second goal was to see if we could discover novel selectivities that either haven't been hypothesized before, or that have remained hidden due to the lower spatial resolution of fMRI data," Khosla says.
To do that, the researchers applied a mathematical method that allows them to discover neural populations that can't be identified from traditional fMRI data. An fMRI image is made up of many voxels — three-dimensional units that represent a cube of brain tissue. Each voxel contains hundreds of thousands of neurons, and if some of those neurons belong to smaller populations that respond to one type of visual input, their responses may be drowned out by other populations within the same voxel.
The new analytical method, which Kanwisher's lab has previously used on fMRI data from the auditory cortex, can tease out responses of neural populations within each voxel of fMRI data.
Using this approach, the researchers found four populations that corresponded to previously identified clusters that respond to faces, places, bodies, and words. "That tells us that this method works, and it tells us that the things that we found before are not just obscure properties of that pathway, but major, dominant properties," Kanwisher says.